Dictionary Definition
aggression
Noun
1 a disposition to behave aggressively
2 a feeling of hostility that arouses thoughts of
attack [syn: aggressiveness]
3 violent action that is hostile and usually
unprovoked [syn: hostility]
4 the act of initiating hostilities
5 deliberately unfriendly behavior
User Contributed Dictionary
see Aggression
English
Etymology
Borrowed from Latin aggressio, from aggressus, past participle of aggredi meaning "to approach, address, attack"Noun
- The act of initiating hostilities or invasion.
- The practice or habit of launching attacks.
- Hostile or destructive behavior or actions.
Derived terms
Related terms
Translations
The act of initiating hostilities or invasion
- Crimean Tatar: tecavuz, agressiya
- Croatian: agresija
- Dutch: agressie
- German: Aggression
- Greek: επιθετικότητα
- Japanese: 侵略 (shinryaku)
- Kurdish:
The practice or habit of launching attacks
- Crimean Tatar: tecavuz, agressiya
- Croatian: agresija
- Dutch: agressie
- German: Aggression
- Japanese: 侵略 (shinryaku)
Hostile or destructive behavior or actions
- Crimean Tatar: tecavuz, agressiya
- Croatian: agresija
- German: Aggression
- Greek: επιθετικότητα
- Japanese: 攻撃 (kōgeki)
- Chinese:
- Simplified Chinese: 侵略
- Traditional Chinese: 侵略
- Pinyin: qinlue
- Traditional Chinese: 侵略
- Simplified Chinese: 侵略
- ttbc French: agression
- ttbc Hebrew: תוקפנות (toqpanut)
- ttbc Italian: aggressione
- Korean:
- hangul: 침략
- romaja: chimnyag
- hangul: 침략
- ttbc Latin: aggressio nominative, aggressionis genitive
- ttbc Maltese: aggressjoni
- Russian:
- Cyrillic:
агрессия
- Roman: agressiya
- Cyrillic:
агрессия
- ttbc Spanish: agresión
Finnish
Noun
aggressionExtensive Definition
- ''For other usage, see Aggression (war crime)
In psychology and other social and
behavioral
sciences, aggression refers to behavior that is intended to
cause harm or pain. Aggression can be either physical or verbal.
Behavior that accidentally causes harm or pain is not aggression.
Property damage and other destructive behavior may also fall under
the definition of aggression. Aggression is not the same thing as
assertiveness.
Different types of aggression
Aggression is a complex phenomenon that is composed of a number of more specific types of behavior.Moyer Classification
Moyer (1968) presented an early and influential classification of seven different forms of aggression, from a biological and evolutionary point of view.- Predatory aggression: attack on prey by a predator.
- Inter-male aggression: competition between males of the same species over access to resources such as females, dominance, status, etc.
- Fear-induced aggression: aggression associated with attempts to flee from a threat.
- Irritable aggression: aggression induced by frustration and directed against an available target.
- Territorial aggression: defence of a fixed area against intruders, typically conspecifics.
- Maternal aggression: a female's aggression to protect her offspring from a threat. Paternal aggression also exists.
- Instrumental aggression: aggression directed towards obtaining some goal, considered to be a learned response to a situation.
Current
Currently, there is a consensus in the scientific community for at least two broad categories of aggression, variously known as hostile, affective, or retaliatory aggression, versus instrumental, predatory, or goal-oriented aggression. Empirical research indicates that this is a critical difference, both psychologically and physiologically. Some research indicates that people with tendencies toward affective aggression have lower IQs than those with tendencies toward predatory aggression.. If the definition of aggression is limited to physical strategies only, then it is true that in some cases, males are more aggressive than females. One explanation for this difference is that females are physically weaker than men so they need to develop other means to obtain successful results and defend themselves . Females of different cultures have a variety of aggressive means through which they can get even with their husbands (e.g., locking them out of the house), all of which could be seen as forms of non-physical aggression. On Bellona Island, a culture that is based on male dominance and physical violence, women tend to get into conflicts with other women more frequently than with men. When they do get into conflicts with their husbands, they rarely use physical means. Instead, they make up songs that mock their husband, which spread across the island, humiliating their husband. If a woman wanted to kill a man, she would either convince her relatives to kill him or hire an assassin. These are both forms of indirect aggression since the aggressor (female) is trying to hurt another individual without putting herself in direct danger .Biology of aggression
See also NeuroplasticityAggression is directed to and often originates
from outside stimuli, but has a very distinct internal character.
Using various techniques and experiments, scientists have been able
to explore the relationships between various parts of the body and
aggression.
Aggression in the brain
Many researchers focus on the brain to explain aggression. The areas involved in aggression in mammals include the amygdala, hypothalamus, prefrontal cortex, cingulate cortex, hippocampus, septal nuclei, and periaqueductal gray of the midbrain. Because of the difficulties in determining the intentions of animals, aggression is defined in neuroscience research as behavior directed at an object or animal which results in damage or harm to that object or animal.The hypothalamus and periaqueductal
gray of the midbrain are the most critical areas controlling
aggression in mammals, as shown in studies on cats, rats, and
monkeys. These brain areas control the expression of all the
behavioral and autonomic components of aggression in these species,
including vocalization. They have direct connections with both the
brainstem nuclei controlling these functions and areas such as the
amygdala and prefrontal cortex.
Electrical stimulation of the hypothalamus causes
aggressive behavior the hypothalamus expresses receptors that help
determine aggression levels based on their interactions with the
neurotransmitters serotonin and vasopressin.
The amygdala is also critically
involved in aggression. Stimulation of the amygdala results in
augmented aggressive behavior in hamsters, while lesions of an
evolutionarily
homologous area in the lizard greatly reduce competitive drive
and aggression(Bauman et al 2006). Several experiments in
attack-primed Syrian Golden Hamsters support the claim of the
amygdala being involved in control of aggression. Using expression
of c-fos as a
neuroanatomically localized marker of activity, the neural
circuitry involved in the state of “attack readiness” in attack
primed hamsters was studied. The results showed that certain
structures of the amygdala were involved in aggressiveness: the
medial nucleus and the cortical nuclei showed distinct differences
in involvement as compared to other structures such as the lateral
and basolateral nuclei and central nucleus of the amygdala, which
were not associated with any substantial changes in aggressiveness.
In addition, c-fos expression was found most clearly in the most
dorsal and caudal aspects of the corticomedial amygdala (CMA). In
the same study, it was also shown that lesions of the CMA
significantly reduced the number of aggressive behaviors. Eight of
eleven subjects failed to attack. Also a correlation between lesion
site and attack latency was determined: the more anterior the
lesion, the longer mean elapsed time to the aggressive behavior
.
The prefrontal
cortex (PFC) has been implicated in aggressive psychopathology.
Reduced activity of the prefrontal cortex, in particular its medial
and orbitofrontal portions, has been associated with
violent/antisocial aggression. Specifically, regulation of the
levels of the neurotransmitter serotonin in the PFC has been
connected with a particular type of pathological aggression,
induced by subjecting genetically predisposed, aggressive,
wild-type mice to repeated winning experience; the male mice
selected from aggressive lines had lower serotonin tissue levels in
the PFC than the low-aggressive lines in this study .
Neurotransmitters and hormones
Various neurotransmitters and hormones have been shown to correlate with aggressive behavior. The most often mentioned of these is the hormone testosterone. In one source, it was noted that concentration of testosterone most clearly correlated with aggressive responses involving provocation. In adulthood, it is clear that testosterone is not related to any consistent methods of measuring aggression on personality scales, but several studies of the concentration of blood testosterone of convicted male criminals who committed violent crimes compared to males without a criminal record or who committed non-aggressive crimes revealed in most cases that men who were judged aggressive/dominant had higher blood concentrations of testosterone than controls. However, a correlation between testosterone levels and aggression does not prove a causal role for testosterone. Studies of testosterone levels of male athletes before and after a competition revealed that testosterone levels rise shortly before their matches, as if in anticipation of the competition, and are dependent on the outcome of the event: testosterone levels of winners are high relative to those of losers. Interestingly, testosterone levels in female criminals versus females without a criminal record mirror those of males: testosterone levels are higher in women who commit aggressive crimes or are deemed aggressive by their peers than non-aggressive females. However, no specific response of testosterone levels to competition was observed in female athletes, although a mood difference was noted. Testosterone has been shown to correlate with aggressive behavior in mice and in some humans, but in contrast to some long-standing theories, various experiments have failed to find a relationship between testosterone levels and aggression in humans. The possible correlation between testosterone and aggression could explain the "roid rage" that can result from anabolic steroid use, although an effect of abnormally high levels of steroids does not prove an effect at physiological levels.Another line of research has focused more on the
effects of circulating testosterone on the nervous system mediated
by local metabolism within the brain. Testosterone can be
metabolized to 17b-estradiol by the enzyme aromatase or to
5a-dihydrotestosterone by 5a-reductase.
Aromatase is highly expressed in regions involved in the regulation
of aggressive behavior, such as the amygdala and hypothalamus. In
studies using genetic knock out techniques in inbred mice, male
mice that lacked a functional aromatase enzyme displayed a marked
reduction in aggression. Long-term treatment of these mice with
estradiol partially restored aggressive behavior, suggesting that
the neural conversion of circulating testosterone to estradiol and
its effect on estrogen receptors affects inter-male aggression.
Also, two different estrogen receptors, ERa and ERb, have been
identified as having the ability to exert different effects on
aggression. In studies using estrogen receptor knockout
mice, individuals lacking a functional ERa displayed markedly
reduced inter-male aggression while male mice that lacked a
functional ERb exhibited normal or slightly elevated levels of
aggressive behavior. These results imply that ERa facilitates
male-male aggression, where as ERb may inhibit aggression. However,
different strains of mice show the opposite pattern in that
aromatase activity is negatively correlated with aggressive
behavior. Also, in a different strain of mice the behavioral effect
of estradiol is
dependent on daylength: under long-days (16h of light) estradiol
reduces aggression, and under short-days (8h of light) estradiol
rapidly increases aggression .
Glucocorticoids
also play an important role in regulating aggressive behavior. In
adult rats, acute injections of corticosterone promote
aggressive behavior and acute reduction of corticosterone decreases
aggression; however, a chronic reduction of corticosterone levels
can produce abnormally aggressive behavior. In addition,
glucocorticoids affect development of aggression and establishment
of social hierarchies. Adult mice with low baseline levels of
corticosterone are more likely to become dominant than are mice
with high baseline corticosterone levels .
Dehydroepiandrosterone
(DHEA) is the most abundant circulating androgen and can be rapidly
metabolized within target tissues into potent androgens and
estrogens. Gonadal steroids generally regulate aggression during
the breeding season, but non-gonadal steroids may regulate
aggression during the non-breeding season. Castration of various
species in the non-breeding season has no effect on territorial
aggression. In several avian studies, circulating DHEA has been
found to be elevated in birds during the non-breeding season. These
data support the idea that non-breeding birds combine adrenal
and/or gonadal DHEA synthesis with neural DHEA metabolism to
maintain territorial behavior when gonadal testosterone secretion
is low. Similar results have been found in studies involving
different strains of rats, mice, and hamsters. DHEA levels also
have been studied in humans and may play a role in human
aggression. Circulating DHEAS (its sulfated ester) levels rise
during adrenarche (~7 years of age) while plasma testosterone
levels are relatively low. This implies that aggression in
pre-pubertal children with aggressive conduct disorder might be
correlated with plasma DHEAS rather than plasma testosterone,
suggesting an important link between DHEAS and human aggressive
behavior .
Another chemical messenger with implications for
aggression is the neurotransmitter serotonin. In various
experiments, serotonin action was shown to be negatively correlated
with aggression(Delville et al. 1997). This correlation with
aggression helps to explain the aggression-reducing effects of
selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors such as fluoxetine (Delville et al.
1997), aka prozac.
While serotonin and testosterone have been the
two most researched chemical messengers with regards to aggression,
other neurotransmitters and hormones have been shown to relate to
aggressive behavior as well. The neurotransmitter vasopressin
causes an increase in aggressive behavior when present in large
amounts in the anterior hypothalamus (Delville et al. 1997). The
effects of norepinephrine, cortisol, and other neurotransmitters
are still being studied.
Genetics and aggression
In a nonmammilian example, the fruitless gene in D. melanogaster is a critical determinant for how fruit flies fight. Patterns of aggression can be switched, with males using female patterns of aggression or females using male patterns, by manipulating either the fruitless or transformer genes in the brain. Candidate genes for differentiating aggression between the sexes are the Sry (sex determining region Y) gene, located on the Y chromosome and the Sts (steroid sulfatase) gene. The Sts gene encodes the steroid sulfatase enzyme, which is pivotal in the regulation of neurosteroid biosynthesis. It is expressed in both sexes, is correlated with levels of aggression among male mice, and increases dramatically in females after parturition and during lactation, corresponding to the onset of maternal aggression.Evolution of aggression
Like most behaviors, aggression can be examined in terms of its ability to help an animal reproduce and survive. Animals may use aggression to gain and secure territories, as well as other resources including food, water, and mating opportunities. Researchers have theorized that aggression and the capacity for murder are products of our evolutionary past.Aggression against outsiders
The most apparent type of aggression is that seen in the interaction between a predator and its prey.An animal defending itself against a predator
becomes aggressive in order to survive and to ensure the survival
of its progeny. Because aggression against a much larger enemy or
group of enemies would lead to the death of an animal, animals have
developed a good sense of when they are outnumbered or outgunned.
This ability to gauge the strength of other animals gives animals a
“fight or flight” response to predators; depending on how strong
they gauge the predator to be, animals will either become
aggressive or flee.
The need to ensure the continuation of one’s
genes leads to a phenomenon known as altruism. An example of an
altruistic act is the alarm call that is given when a predator is
approaching. While this call will inform the community of a
predator’s presence, it will also inform the predator of the
whereabouts of the animal that gave the alarm call. While this
would appear to give the alarm caller an evolutionary disadvantage,
it would facilitate the continuation of this animal’s genes because
it's relatives and progeny would be more able to avoid
predators.
According to many researchers, predation is not
aggression. Cats do not hiss or arch their backs when in pursuit of
a rat, and the active areas in their hypothalamuses are more
similar to those that reflect hunger than those that reflect
aggression.
Aggression within a species
Aggression against conspecifics serves a number of purposes having to do with breeding. One of the most common of these purposes is the establishment of a dominance hierarchy. When certain types of animals are first placed in a common environment, the first thing they do is fight to assert their role in the dominance hierarchy. In general, the more dominant animals will be more aggressive than their subordinates.. The majority of conspecific aggression ceases about 24 hours after the introduction of the animals being tested.There are many different theories that try to
explain how males and females developed these different aggressive
tendencies. One theory states that in species where one sex makes a
higher parental investment than the other, the higher investing sex
is a resource for which the other sex competes: in the majority of
species, females are the higher investing sex. It also holds that
the main objective of survival is to have the chance to pass on an
individual’s genetic
material. For males, it is of crucial importance to establish
dominance and resource holding to obtain reproductive opportunities
in order to pass on their genetics. Unlike females, whose
reproductive success is constrained by long gestation and lactation periods, male
reproductive success is constrained by the number of partners they
can mate with. As a result, males employ physical aggression more
often than females; they take more risks in order to compete with
other males and gain an elevation of status. Males even go as far
as killing one another and offspring belonging to other males.
Males demonstrate less concern for their physical welfare in such
competitions. In contrast, females compete with one another for
resources, which can be converted to offspring. The establishment
of dominance is more costly for females than for males and females
have less to gain from achieving status. The female presence is
more critical to the offspring’s survival and hence her
reproductive success than is the father’s. It is only logical then
that the health and well being of females would cause them to use
less aggressive, low risk, and indirect strategies to acquire
resources. As a result, in the majority of female-female conflicts,
females rarely inflict serious damage to one another over
resources. When translated to human, these facts suggest that women
should be expected to show less evidence of dominance hierarchies
than men do. In society, aggression in boys becomes increasingly
motivated by issues of social status and self-esteem, which are
usually decided by varying degrees of aggressive reactivity to
personal challenge. Aggression in girls, focusing mainly on
resource acquisition and not status, is more likely to take less
physically dangerous and more covert forms of indirect aggression .
There are, however, extensive critiques of the use of animal
behavior to explain human behavior and the application of
evolutionary explanations of contemporary human behavior.
Aggression in humans
Although humans are similar to non-human animals
in some aspects of aggression, they differ from most of these
animals in the complexity of their aggression because of factors
such as culture, morals, and social situations. A wide variety of
studies have been done on these situations.
Aggression and culture
Culture is a distinctly human factor that plays a role in aggression. Anthropological research has found that some cultures are relatively low on aggression, such as the Kung Bushmen, who were described as the "harmless people" by Elizabeth Marshall Thomas (1958). Other researchers, however, have countered this point of view, calculating that the homicide rate among Bushmen is actually higher than that of most modern industrial societies (Keeley, 1996). Lawrence Keeley argues that the "peaceful savage" is a myth that is unsupported by the bulk of anthropological and archeological evidence. Hunter gatherer societies do not have possessions to fight over, but they may still come to conflict over status and mating opportunities.Empirical
cross-cultural research has found differences in the level of
aggression between cultures. In one study, American men resorted to
physical aggression more readily than Japanese or Spanish men,
whereas Japanese men preferred direct verbal conflict more than
their American and Spanish counterparts (Andreu et al. 1998).
Within American culture, southerners
were shown to become more aroused and to respond more aggressively
than northerners when affronted (Bowdle et al. 1996). There is also
a higher homicide rate among young white southern men than among
white northern men in the United States (Nisbett 1993). Changes in
dominant behavior or in social status causes changes in
testosterone levels. Reports of changes in testeosterone of young
men during athletic events, which involve face-to-face competition
with a winner and a loser, reveal that testosterone rises shortly
before their matches, as if in anticipation of the competition.
Also, one to two hours after the competitive match, the
testosterone levels of the winners are high relative to those
levels of the losers . It is also important to take into account
the type of conflict that is occurring when assessing aggression.
Is the conflict between groups, within a group, within a family?
The sex of those involved in the conflict is also critical.
Male-male, male-female and female-female encounters should all be
clearly distinguished from one another. Same sex encounters are
more frequent than inter-sex encounters and this could affect the
level of aggression present .
Aggression in media
main article Media violence researchBehaviors like aggression can be learned by
watching and imitating the behavior of others. A considerable
amount of evidence suggests that watching violence on television
increases the likelihood of violent behavior in children (Aronson,
Wilson, & Akert, 2005), though for a dissenting viewpoint, see
Freedman (2002). Individuals may differ in how they respond to
violence. The greatest impact is on those who are already prone to
violent behavior. Adults may be influenced by violence in media as
well. A long-term study of over 700 families found "a significant
association" between the amount of time spent watching violent
television as a teenager and the likelihood of committing acts of
aggression later in life. The results remained the same in spite of
factors such as family income, parental education and neighborhood
violence (Aronson, Wilson, & Akert, 2005).
Although exposure to violence in media is
associated with the risk for violent behavior, none of these
studies provide evidence for a definitive causal mechanism.
Instead, violence in media may be one of many factors, or it may
play a maintenance role since violent media tend to be selected by
people who are prone to violence.
Situational factors
see also Stereotype threat Alcohol impairs judgment, making people much less cautious than they usually are (MacDonald et al. 1996). It also disrupts the way information is processed (Bushman 1993, 1997; Bushman & Cooper 1990). A drunk person is much more likely to view an accidental event as a purposeful one, and therefore act more aggressively. Pain and discomfort also increase aggression. Even the simple act of placing ones hands in cold water can cause an aggressive response. Hot temperatures have been implicated as a factor in a number of studies. One study completed in the midst of the civil rights movement found that riots were more likely on hotter days than cooler ones (Carlsmith & Anderson 1979). Students were found to be more aggressive and irritable after taking a test in a hot classroom (Anderson et al. 1996, Rule, et al. 1987). Drivers in cars without air conditioning were also found to be more likely to honk their horns (Kenrick & MacFarlane 1986).Frustration is another major cause of aggression.
The frustration-aggression theory states that aggression increases
if a person feels that he or she is being blocked from achieving a
goal (Aronson et al. 2005). One study found that the closeness to
the goal makes a difference. The study examined people waiting in
line and concluded that the 2nd person was more aggressive than the
12th one when someone cut in line (Harris 1974). Unexpected
frustration may be another factor. In a separate study, a group of
students were collecting donations over the phone. Some of them
were told that the people they would call would be generous and the
collection would be very successful. The other group was given no
expectations. The group with high expectations was much more upset
and became more aggressive when no one was pledging (Kulik &
Brown 1979).
There is some evidence to suggest that the
presence of violent objects such as a gun can trigger aggression.
In a study done by Leonard Berkowitz and Anthony Le Page (1967),
college students were made angry and then left in the presence of a
gun or badminton racket. They were then led to believe they were
delivering electric shocks to another student, as in the Milgram
experiment. Those who had been in the presence of the gun
administered more shocks. It is possible that a violence-related
stimulus increases the likelihood of aggressive cognitions by
activating the semantic
network.
Aggression and gender
Gender is a factor that plays a role in both human and animal aggression. Males are generally more physically aggressive than females (Coi & Dodge 1997, Maccoby & Jacklin 1974), and men commit the vast majority of murders (Buss 2005). This is one of the most robust and reliable behavioral sex differences, and it has been found across many different age groups and cultures. There is evidence that males are quicker to aggression (Frey et al 2003) and more likely than females to express their aggression physically (Bjorkqvist et al. 1994). However, some researchers have suggested that females are not necessarily less aggressive, but that they tend to show their aggression in less overt, less physical ways (Bjorkqvist et al. 1994, Hines and Saudino 2003). For example, females may display more verbal and relational aggression, such as social rejection.Aggression in children
The frequency of physical aggression in humans peaks at around 2-3 years of age. It then declines gradually on average (Tremblay 2000). These observations suggest that physical aggression is mostly not a learned behavior and that development provides opportunities for the learning of self-regulation. However, a small subset of children fails to acquire the necessary self-regulatory abilities and tends to show atypical levels of physical aggression across development (Bongers et al. 2004; National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Early Child Care Research Network 2004). These may be at risk for later violent behavior.- Young children preparing to enter kindergarten need to develop the socially important skill of being assertive. Examples of assertiveness include asking others for information, initiating conversation, or being able to respond to peer pressure.
- In contrast, some young children use aggressive behavior, such as hitting or biting, as a form of communication.
- Aggressive behavior can impede learning as a skill deficit, while assertive behavior can facilitate learning.
- By school age, children should learn more socially appropriate forms of communicating such as expressing themselves through verbal or written language; if they have not, this behavior may signify a disability or developmental delay
- Physical fear of others
- Family difficulties
- Learning, neurological, or conduct/behavior disorders
- Emotional trauma
The Bobo
doll experiment was conducted by Albert Bandura in 1961. In
this work, Bandura found that children exposed to aggressive adult
model acted more aggressively than those who were exposed to a
nonaggressive adult model. This experiment suggests that anyone who
comes in contact with and interacts with children can have an
impact on the way they react and handle situations..
(1) American Academy of Pediatrics : 'Set firm,
consistent limits to help children self monitor emotions and
behavior; make sure all care takers agree to the same limits.
Provide examples of effective and socially acceptable ways of
managing anger; be careful not to reinforce aggression with
aggressive forms of punishment. Also, model acceptable behavior as
a caretaker by managing your own temper. Remember that occasional
outbursts are normal. If aggressive behavior continues for more
than a few weeks, consult a pediatrician or mental health
professional.'http://www.aap.org/
(2) National Association of School Psychologists:
'Overly aggressive behavior can signify a social skills deficit;
direct instruction, modeling, and coaching can help children
acquire the skill of assertion, which as a replacement behavior may
help prevent aggressive behavior.' (NASP Best Practices in School
Psychology 2002):http://nasponline.org/
War and sports
Two basic models of the relationship between war and sports have been identified: the drive discharge model and the culture pattern model. According to the drive discharge model, tendency towards aggression is innate, a buildup of aggressive tension causes warfare, and warlike sports make war less likely by providing an alternative outlet for aggressive tension. The culture pattern model holds that the intensity and configuration of aggressive behavior is acquired from the surrounding culture, aggression levels are consistent across multiple areas of a culture, and behaviors and attitudes concerning war tend to match those concerning warlike sports. The drive discharge model suggests that levels of aggression do not vary across societies, although the type of aggressive behavior can vary, with an inverse relationship between the propensity to war and to warlike sports. The culture pattern model predicts that levels of aggression vary across societies, with a direct relationship between the propensity to war and to warlike sports.See also
Notes
References
- Akert, M. Robin, Aronson, E., and Wilson, D.T. "Social Psychology", 5th Edition. Pearson Education, Inc. 2005.
- Amaral, D.G., M D. Bauman, P Lavenex, W A. Mason, and J. E. Toscano. "The Expression of Social Dominance Following Neonatal Lesions of the Amygdala or Hippocampus in Rhesus Monkeys (Macaca Mulatta)." Behavioral Neuroscience 120 (2006): 749-760. 7 Dec. 2006 <http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=pubmed&cmd=Retrieve&dopt=AbstractPlus&list_uids=16893283&query_hl=3&itool=pubmed_docsum>.
- Andreu, J. Manuel, Takehiro Fujihara, Takaya Kohyama, and J. Martin Ramirez. "Justification of Interpersonal Aggression in Japanese, American, and Spanish Students." Aggressive Behavior 25 (1998): 185-195. 7 Dec. 2006 .
- Anderson, C.A., Dill, K.E.. "Video Games and Aggressive Thoughts, Feelings, and Behavior in the Laboratory and in Life." Journal of Personality and Social Psychology (2000), Vol. 78, No. 4, 772-790 .
- Bjorkqvist, Kaj, Kirsti M. Lagerspetz, and Karin Osterman. "Sex Differences in Covert Aggression." Aggressive Behavior 202 (1994): 27-33. 6 Dec. 2006 .
- Bongers, I. L., Koot, H. M., van der Ende, J., & Verhulst, F. C. (2004). Developmental trajectories of externalizing behaviors in childhood and adolescence. Child Development, 75, 1523-1537.
- Bowdle, Brian F., Dov Cohen, Richerd E. Nisbett, and Norbert Schwarz. "Insult, Aggression, and the Southern Culture of Honor: an “Experimental." Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 70 (1996): 945-960. 7 Dec. 2006 <http://www.som.yale.edu/faculty/keith.chen/negot.%20papers/CohenNisbettEtAll2_SouthCultureHonor96.pdf>.
- Coie, J. D. & Dodge, K. A. (1997). Aggression and antisocial behavior. In W. Damon & N. Eisenberg (Eds). Handbook of Child Psychology, Vol. 3: Social, emotional and personality development.
- Dewsbury, Donald A. "Dominance Rank, Copulatory Behavior, and Differential Reproduction." Quarterly Review of Biology 57 (1982): 135-159. 7 Dec. 2006 .
- Figler, M H., J E. Finkelstein, H. V. S. Peeke, and M Twum. "Maternal Aggression in Red Swamp Crayfish (Procambarus Clarkii, Girard): the Relation Between Reproductive Status and Outcome of Aggressive Encounters with Male and Female Conspecifics." Behaviour 132 (1995): 107-125. 7 Dec. 2006 .
- Freedman, J. (2002). Media violence and its effect on aggression.: Assessing the scientific evidence. Toronto: University of Toronto Press.
- Hines, Denise A., and Kimberly J. Saudino. "Gender Differences in Psychological, Physical, and Sexual Aggression Among College Students Using the Revised Conflict Tactics Scales." Violence and Victims 18 (2003): 197-217. 7 Dec. 2006 .
- Keeley, L. H. (1996). War before civilization: The myth of the peaceful savage. New York: Oxford University Press.
- Maccoby. E. E. & Jacklin. C.N. (1974). The psychology of sex differences, Stanford: Stanford University Press.
- Maestripieri, D. "Functional Aspects of Maternal Aggression in Mammals." Canadian Journal of Zoology 70 (1992): 1069-1077. 7 Dec. 2006 Functional Aspects of Maternal Aggression in Mammals.
- National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Early Child Care Research Network (2004). Trajectories of physical aggression from toddlerhood to middle childhood: Predictors, correlates, and outcomes. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 69(4), vii-128.
- Nisbett, R. E. (1993). Violence and U.S. regional culture. American Psychologist, 48, 441-449.
- Silverberg, James; J. Patrick Gray (1992) Aggression and Peacefulness in Humans and Other Primates ISBN 0195071190
- Somit, A (1990) Humans, chimps, and bonobos: The biological bases of aggression, war, and peacemaking. Journal of Conflict Resolution 34:-582
- Thomas, E. M. (1958). The harmless people. New York: Vintage Books.
- Tremblay, R. E. (2000). The development of aggressive behaviour during childhood: What have we learned in the past century? International Journal of Behavioral Development, 24, 129-141.
- Developmental Origins of Aggression
aggression in Bosnian: Agresija
aggression in Bulgarian: Агресия
(психология)
aggression in Czech: Agrese
aggression in German: Aggression
aggression in Spanish: Agresión
aggression in Esperanto: Agreso
aggression in Persian: پرخاشگری
aggression in French: Agression
aggression in Galician: Agresión
aggression in Croatian: Agresija
aggression in Indonesian: Agresi
aggression in Latvian: Agresija
aggression in Lithuanian: Agresija
aggression in Hungarian: Agresszió
aggression in Dutch: Agressie
aggression in Japanese: 攻撃行動
aggression in Norwegian: Aggresjon
aggression in Polish: Agresja
(psychologia)
aggression in Portuguese: Agressividade
aggression in Quechua: Ch'utiy
aggression in Russian: Агрессия
(психология)
aggression in Sicilian: Aggrissioni
aggression in Slovak: Agresia
(psychológia)
aggression in Serbian: Агресија (људско
понашање)
aggression in Finnish: Aggressio
aggression in Tagalog: Paghandulong
aggression in Ukrainian: Агресія
aggression in Chinese: 侵略
Synonyms, Antonyms and Related Words
adventuresomeness,
adventurousness,
aggravated assault, aggressiveness, ambitiousness, amphibious
attack, antagonism,
armed assault, assailing, assailment, assault, attack, banzai attack, bellicism, bellicosity, belligerence, belligerency, blitz, blitzkrieg, breakthrough, charge, chauvinism, combativeness, contentiousness,
counterattack,
counteroffensive, coup
de main, crippling attack, dead set at, descent on, diversion, diversionary
attack, drive, dynamism, encroachment, enterprise, enterprisingness,
ferocity, fierceness, fight, flank attack, force, forcefulness, frontal
attack, gas attack, get-up-and-get, get-up-and-go, getup, go, go-ahead, go-getting,
go-to-itiveness, gumption, head-on attack,
hostility, hustle, incursion, infiltration, initiative, inroad, invasion, irruption, jingoism, lightning attack,
lightning war, martialism, mass attack,
megadeath, militancy, militarism, mugging, offense, offensive, onset, onslaught, overkill, panzer warfare,
pugnaciousness,
pugnacity, push, pushfulness, pushiness, pushingness, quarrelsomeness,
raid, run against, run at,
rush, saber rattling,
sally, shock tactics,
sortie, spirit, spunk, strike, truculence, unfriendliness, unpeacefulness,
unprovoked assault, up-and-comingness, venturesomeness,
venturousness,
warmongering,
warpath